What type of person likes scary movies




















Save Pin FB More. Something went wrong. An error has occurred and your entry was not submitted. Please try again. View Series. Close Sign in. Here's how. Body language is an external signal of a person's emotional state. Learn about types of body language and how to read them. We've gathered the latest data to see how many people cheat, if they get away with it, and what it can do to your mental health.

It may be embarrassing for you, indulgent even, or just part of your weekly routine, but here's what research has to show about masturbation and your….

Sapiosexuality is a sexual identity describing those who are aroused by intelligence. Learn how sapiosexuality may effect relationships. Love addiction is a controversial term. Still, what you feel is real. Here's what research and experts say it means and why it may not be addiction…. Message intended not being the message received time and again? Check out our evidence-based pointers for how to communicate better.

You two aren't the only ones; trust us. See what problems are common in marriage communication, plus how to resolve them, starting today. All About Toxic Masculinity. Proximity to a parent is perceived as being the most successful fear-reduction coping strategy in young children Wilson et al. Very young children under 2 years experience less fear through covering their eyes; in 3—5-year olds, this behavior increases fear Wilson, Cognitive strategies, such as talking about films and programs with parents or other adults, have been found to be effective Cantor and Wilson, By far, the most common type of cognitive strategy employed by parents is reassuring children that the stimulus children are afraid of does not exist Cantor and Hoffner, , although this is likely to be successful in older children but not in younger children 4—5 years; Cantor and Wilson, Explaining that the source of fear is not likely to be harmful is also successful in older 8—9 year old children Wilson and Cantor, Verbal explanations may be ineffective in younger children who are less likely to discuss horror materials with their parents.

If children are informed that a film has a happy ending, they report less fear Hoffner and Cantor, ; Hoffner, If children rehearse verbal information e. Children also regard the spiders as less dangerous after being given these instructions. Two physical means of coping with frightening stimuli studied in children are blunting avoiding threat or transforming a threat by distraction; looking away, for example and monitoring being action oriented and attending to the threat.

Sparks and Spirek found that high blunters and low monitors were less physiologically aroused by horror films than were high monitors and low blunters suggesting that underlying physiology might predict or predispose individuals to react in a given emotional way to frightening stimuli; Sparks a , b also found that low monitors were less negative about horror when given information about the film but this information had no effect on blunters.

A study of 14— and 15—year olds examined the role of blunting and monitoring on coping with scary films Hoffner, Davis and Kraus had previously reported that high empathetic concern was associated with less loneliness and unsociability; high personal distress was associated with shyness, poor interpersonal functioning, and social anxiety. Empathetic concern was found to encourage altruism, whereas personal distress prompted people to reduce their own emotion expression Batson, Hoffner found a series of interesting results.

A belief that something was unreal was the most common coping strategy, followed by interpersonal comfort and momentary avoidance; these were used more than was distraction. The study found that boys preferred scary films more than did girls, a finding consistent with the literature, that girls reported more empathetic concern and personal distress, that personal distress correlated with empathy and with monitoring and blunting, that these correlated negatively with liking for scary films, that blunting predicted use of distraction and unreality, that monitoring was more widely used and was more effective, that monitoring and blunting were associated with increased interpersonal comfort, that girls were more likely to use momentary avoidance and interpersonal comfort and consider them more effective, that people who reported using one strategy were more likely to use all four, that empathy, but not personal distress, was associated with greater use of reality, IC and personal distress were associated with increased use of distraction, and that higher empathy scores were associated with greater use of Unreality.

People who liked horror were less likely to use distraction, unreality, and momentary avoidance as coping strategies, which suggest that coping is related to the dislike of horror — it is something that must be done to mitigate the effects of something that is disliked. If people thought the coping strategies worked, they enjoyed the films more. Hoffner also noted that participants who reported finding scary films and television to be violent were likely to use all four coping mechanisms; those who found the material to be realistic were more likely to report using distraction, unreality, and interpersonal comfort as coping mechanisms.

Material featuring blood and gore was more likely to lead to the use of momentary avoidance. Girls reported using momentary avoidance and interpersonal comfort more than did boys and considered these to be more effective strategies than did boys.

As children enter adolescence, their reasons for seeking out horror develop and change — they will watch to be thrilled, to rebel because parents have prohibited them , or to enjoy gore because they are interested in how people die Oliver, a , b.

One study of 13—year-old boys and girls examined their motivation for watching slasher movies Johnston, Reasons for watching included gore watching, thrill watching, an increased feeling of independence bravery, and problem avoidance. Thrill watching and independence were positively related to positive affect; positive views of slashers were associated with high gore and thrill watching and gore watching predicted preference for graphic violence. Boys were more likely to watch graphic horror because they were motivated to seek out gore, and they were also more likely to identify with the killer than were girls; girls were more likely to identify with the victim.

A larger survey of 6, 10—year-old US adolescents in found similar sex differences: watching violent films was associated with being male, older, non-white, having less educated parents, and having poor school achievement Worth et al. Both boys and girls who found violent cartoons funny and thrilling also scored higher on neuroticism, psychoticism, and sensation seeking.

The majority of the research on the development of horror preference and response to horror film has recruited children and adolescents as participants. There is some, but not much, research on how older people respond to horror, and this suggests that the preference for horror declines with age Tamborini and Stiff, ; Hoffner and Levine, Clasen et al.

As Clasen et al. The average age of those who agreed that they strongly liked horror media was slightly lower than those who disagreed They also note that since sensation seeking also declines with age, this might explain the reduction in enjoyment and seeking out of horror with increasing age post adolescence. Children tend to express greater fear to different types of stimuli and content depending on the age of the child.

There are also differences between boys and girls and between age groups in the types of coping strategies they adopt during and after watching frightening television and film material. One of the principal purposes of horror film is to induce fear. The nature of fear and its etiology has a long history in psychology, and various models have been proposed, which have attempted to explain why we become afraid and to what types of stimulus. The module has four features: it is selective, it is automatic when encountering fear-relevant stimuli, it responds without mediation , it is encapsulated i.

It is considered to be an adaptive mechanism for allowing us to avoid physical danger rapidly Schaller and Neuberg, In the context of horror film, this is, of course, counter-intuitive as horror film viewers who enjoy horror may not wish to escape the horror and deliberately and proactively approach and seek it, and those that do not enjoy horror and who may serendipitously watch horror engage in other withdrawal behaviors such as shutting the eyes or holding on to a companion they may also leave a cinema or turn off a screen.

The questions that then arise are whether there are specific stimuli or situations, which horror films deploy or recruit which are more likely to induce a fear response and, if so, what are these stimuli and why do they have this effect. The latter stimuli pose no immediate and real physical threat to survival i.

These stimuli and situations were those which once posed threats to our ancestors and that we, therefore, developed an evolutionary disposition to avoid or to respond with fear, a form of selective association. Guns, for example, are not fatal unless used, and our exposure to them is limited; guns are not phobic stimuli and seeing photographs of guns — or seeing guns — does not elicit significant fear, and not the degree of fear that stimuli to which we are evolutionarily predisposed to fear evoke.

A person pointing a gun at us, however, with the intention to fire or with the threat of the intention to fire is clearly a direct threat but not one that is evolutionarily created. One of most common phobias is arachnophobia, and spiders have been a staple of horror films since the s, although only 0. This predisposition facilitates vigilance occasionally, over-vigilance and we see threat in ambiguous situations to sources of threat or danger with greater attention paid to some stimuli Clasen, ; March et al.

It is a self-protection and survival-enabling mechanism motivating us to confront and, therefore, remove the potential source of threat or flee thereby, removing us from the context in which a threat could result in endangerment.

Fear is related to expressions of disgust, and the literature on phobia suggests that the strength of fear for phobic objects is closely related to disgust sensitivity but not trait anxiety Davey, such that people who express abnormal fear of an object also show high degrees of sensitivity to disgusting stimuli but are not dispositionally, highly anxious. A specific phobia, which appears to be qualitatively and quantitatively different from others and is relevant in the context of horror film, is the fear of blood or blood-injection-injury phobia Wani et al.

Individuals experience fear, anxiety, and disgust and avoid or decline medical treatment because of the strength of their phobic reaction Wani et al. This extreme experience may explain why some people feel squeamish at the sight of blood in horror: blood is unique as a stimulus, which evokes a strong fear or disgust reaction.

Fear is the most widely studied emotion in science because it can be easily conditioned, studied, and observed in non-human organisms. There is a substantial literature, which has attempted to explain fear conditioning and learning through reference to its underlying neuropsychology, and much of this work has been conducted on non-human species LeDoux and Hofmann, In humans, much of our understanding of the neurology of fear has derived from neuroimaging research and studies of brain injury.

One of the brain regions involved in fear recognition and experience is the amygdala Martin, ; March et al. No study has specifically examined the effect of exposure to horror film on brain activation, although hundreds of studies have examined the effect of exposure of fear-related stimuli, including films designed to induce fear, on brain activation measured via MEG, PET, fMRI, and EEG. Many studies have examined the consequence of brain injury on the fear response, and one study is especially relevant to horror film as it examined the effect of bilateral amygdala injury on responses to fear-related stimuli in a film-related context Feinstein et al.

In this study, a year-old woman with normal IQ and language showed impaired fear conditioning, impaired recognition of fear in faces, and impaired social-related fear. Feinstein et al.

Although she verbally indicated avoidance of the spiders she physically approached them and asked 15 times if she could touch one; at the haunted house a visitor attraction , she volunteered to lead a group of visitors, did not hesitate in walking around, and was not scared by the monsters she scared the actors. None of the 10 horror film clips elicited fear other film clips designed to elicit other emotions successfully elicited those emotions and she asked for the name of one so that she could rent it.

She recognized that most people would be scared by them. This is only comprehensive study of the effect of region-relevant brain injury on the perception of horror films and horror-related stimuli in a single-case study, and while single case studies need to be interpreted cautiously, the study does provide the opening for other studies to confirm the role of these structures in horror appreciation. One possible extension of this study would be to examine whether amygdala reactivity is associated with enjoyment of horror film those with highly reactive amygdalae may fear or enjoy horror more than those with less reactive amygdalae or whether the amygdala becomes increasingly active with greater stimulation, and the intensity of the experience correlates with the increase in activity while watching.

The current review sought to determine why people watch horror film and how exposure to horror film affects behavior. The conclusions in the previous paragraph are based on a very limited set of data. The studies from which such data have been drawn have varied in sample size, methodology, and materials, and these are three clearly identifiable and major limitations in this field.

Hoffner and Levine have highlighted similar limitations in their meta-analysis. Studies have used a variety — although a very restricted variety — of horror films over 30 years of research, and the films share little in common apart from being classed as horror film. The Silence of the Lambs, Cannibal Holocaust, The Babadook, Saw, The Blair Witch Project, Psycho, Dracula , and The Devil Rides Out are all horror films, but each has distinctive mechanisms of evoking fear and disgust based on story, film making, plot, characters, sound, performance, visual effects, credibility, and use of music.

No one study can fully take into account our response to horror because not all horror films are the same Oliver, a , b , and this limitation needs to be more clearly recognized and addressed in future work. Hoffner and Levine have concluded that the nature of the media content in these studies can explain the failure to find homogeneity in the correlations between enjoyment of horror media and empathic concern in their meta-analysis.

As noted earlier, when correlations were found for empathy and horror enjoyment, the most consistent correlations found were in those studies in which victimization formed the dominant aspect of the horror stimuli. When these studies were removed, the correlations for the remaining studies fell to almost zero. The former limitation can be easily resolved via empirical research. This is not to say that some of these elements have not been studied — this review and others have described studies in which they have — but there has been little research which has examined these elements systematically and methodically, and some elements have not been explored at all.

It is possible to study non-verbal measures such as movement, EEG, brain activation, GSR, and so on , but these are indirect, correlational measures of what an individual might be thinking or feeling. Motor behavior, however, may be a very informative indicator of response to horror, as some of the studies reviewed here suggest. Given the current accessibility of film and media generally via smartphones, as well as internet-ready TVs and, of course, computers, one topic of research that has been little studied is whether the medium affects the perception and enjoyment of horror films.

Screen size and its effect on the enjoyment of displayed material have been relatively well-studied see, for example, Grabe et al. In the context of horror, however, it is hypothesizable that increased screen size leads to increased visibility and that this would result in a stronger fright reaction because more of the horror can be seen and seen more clearly. It is also possible that the augmentation of the screen would also augment the sound an auditory-sound illusion so that bigger screens might affect our perception of horror because of this visual illusion.

There is also scope for further research on coping with the effects of watching horror film and of mitigating the fright if the experience is considered too intense or too unmanageable. Of course, individuals could choose not to watch or could chose to watch selectively if they are in front of the screen. But there may be more imaginative strategies that might be adopted such as the introduction of non-visual, non-verbal, and non-auditory stimuli e.

It is possible that the presence of a pleasant scent might alleviate some of the fright generated by horror film if such alleviation is required either because it distracts or because it creates or elevates positive mood. There is some evidence that this might be possible Martin, , and this is a question that merits pursuit. The majority of the studies reviewed here has included mono-cultural samples, and the current review was unable to uncover any cross-cultural research on horror enjoyment or preference.

An understanding of the cultural influences on film preference especially horror and the individual differences that may underpin them warrants investigation given that certain genres of horror appear to be more popular and appear more often, in specific cultures: Different cultures place different emphases on certain types of content and Japanese horror with its emphasis on ghosts, the supernatural is an obvious example Balmain, ; McRoy, There is a considerable literature on the difference between collectivistic and individualistic cultures with research suggesting that the psychological responses of individuals from each type of cultural background are different Matsumoto et al.

In the field of horror film perception, experience, and enjoyment, it could be hypothesized that individuals from collectivistic cultures might respond differently to horror and victims in horror than do individuals from individualistic cultures — specifically individuals from collectivistic cultures may express greater fear compared to those from individualistic cultures — and this is an hypothesis that can be easily tested.

With interest and appreciation in horror increasing, the scope for undertaking research into horror film has never been more timely. There is still much to discover and still much to understand. Horror, said Adorno in another context, was beyond the scope of psychology. Without psychology, Dario Argento once said, the horror film does not exist. The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest. The author would like to thank Dr Charlie Allbright, Phil Hughes, and four reviewers, especially reviewer 2, for their detailed and thoughtful comments on earlier drafts of this paper, and to Edward Lionheart for planting the seed for this review.

National Center for Biotechnology Information , U. Journal List Front Psychol v. Front Psychol. Published online Oct Author information Article notes Copyright and License information Disclaimer.

Neil Martin, ku. This article was submitted to Cognition, a section of the journal Frontiers in Psychology. Received Feb 7; Accepted Sep The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author s and the copyright owner s are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice.

No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms. This article has been cited by other articles in PMC. Abstract Why do we watch and like horror films? Keywords: horror, terror, fear, film, cinema. Sound in Horror In addition to the visual and verbal dialogue impact of horror, perhaps one of the most significant elements of horror film is auditory. Why do People Watch Horror? Sensation Seeking The most widely studied trait in the research on horror is sensation seeking.

Empathy Empathy is a multidimensional concept whose components have been defined in different ways but which in general are reflected in two types: a cognitive component e. Need for Affect A different form of individual difference — need for affect — may also mediate horror film preference and enjoyment, but the literature is limited. Other Personality Traits Other personality traits thought to be implicated in horror film preference or enjoyment include the Big Five, the Dark Tetrad, and repressive coping style.

Coping How children cope with horror has been the subject of some research on child development and horror because of the potentially harmful psychological consequences of exposure to frightening stimuli. Adolescence As children enter adolescence, their reasons for seeking out horror develop and change — they will watch to be thrilled, to rebel because parents have prohibited them , or to enjoy gore because they are interested in how people die Oliver, a , b.

Aging and Horror Enjoyment The majority of the research on the development of horror preference and response to horror film has recruited children and adolescents as participants.

What Causes Fear? Neuropsychology and Horror Film Fear is the most widely studied emotion in science because it can be easily conditioned, studied, and observed in non-human organisms. Conclusions The current review sought to determine why people watch horror film and how exposure to horror film affects behavior. Limitations and Future Directions The conclusions in the previous paragraph are based on a very limited set of data. Author Contributions The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments The author would like to thank Dr Charlie Allbright, Phil Hughes, and four reviewers, especially reviewer 2, for their detailed and thoughtful comments on earlier drafts of this paper, and to Edward Lionheart for planting the seed for this review.

References Allmer P. Allmer P. NY: Columbia University Press;. Cultural differences in attention: eye movement evidence from a comparative visual search task.

Sex differences in disgust: why are women more easily disgusted than men? Personality and curiosity about TV and films violence in adolescents. Viewing of mass media violence, perception of violence, personality and academic achievement. Startle reflex and emotion modulation impairment after a right amygdala lesion. Brain , — Phobic anxiety in 11 nations: part II. The startle effect. Film Q. Horror films: tales to master terror or shapers of trauma? Introduction to Japanese horror film.

Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 17 , 69— Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press. Cutting film violence: effects on perceptions, enjoyment, and arousal.

Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with activity in paralimbic brain regions. Dawson M. Cambridge: CUP; , — When good things go bad: the reflex physiology of defense. Psychophysiology 55 :e Initial and sustained brain responses to threat anticipation in blood-injection-injury phobia. NeuroImage Clin.

Additive effects of threat-of-shock and picture valence on startle reflex modulation. PLoS One 8 :e An empirical investigation of effects of film music using qualitative content analysis.

Music Cogn. CEUR workshop proceedings. Cantor J. Poetics Today 25 , — Descriptions of media-induced fright reactions in a sample of US elementary school children. Media 4 , 1— Media 34 , — Rogers vs. Media 32 , — Modifying fear responses to mass media in preschool and elementary school children. Helping children cope with frightening media presentations. Emotional responses to a televised nuclear holocaust film.

Effect of forewarning on emotional responses to a horror film. This means they may have a different or more intense physiological reaction to violent or scary movies , HSP researchers say. News U. Politics Joe Biden Congress Extremism. Special Projects Highline. HuffPost Personal Video Horoscopes.



0コメント

  • 1000 / 1000